Poster No:
2470
Submission Type:
Abstract Submission
Authors:
J. Amaro-Fuenzalida1,2, Tomás Ossandón Valdés3,4, Aurelio Cortese5, Ranganatha Sitaram6
Institutions:
1Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Medicina, Santiago de Chile, Chile, 2Neurodynamic of Cognition Laboratory, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, 3Neurodynamic of Cognition Laboratory, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Pontificia Universidad Católica d, Santiago de Chile, Chile, 4Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Medicina, Santiago, Chile, 5Department of Decoded Neurofeedback, ATR Computational Neuroscience Laboratories, Kyoto, Japan, 6St. Jude Children's Research Hospital, Memphis, TN
First Author:
J. Amaro-Fuenzalida, PhD(c)
Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Medicina|Neurodynamic of Cognition Laboratory, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
Santiago de Chile, Chile|Santiago, Chile
Co-Author(s):
Tomás Ossandón Valdés, PhD
Neurodynamic of Cognition Laboratory, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Pontificia Universidad Católica d|Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Medicina
Santiago de Chile, Chile|Santiago, Chile
Aurelio Cortese, PhD
Department of Decoded Neurofeedback, ATR Computational Neuroscience Laboratories
Kyoto, Japan
Introduction:
Consciousness as content refers to the experience of objects, commonly known as awareness (Searle, 2007). The Global Neuronal Workspace Model (GNWM) suggests that perceptual awareness relies on the activation of the fronto-parietal network (Mashour, 2020). Evidence indicates a frontal contribution to the awareness process when subjects report their experiences (Del Cul, 2007). However, GNWM does not explain how conscious contents are represented but focuses on the mechanisms required to access them. Theories like High Order Theory (HOT) address representation, proposing frontal meta-representation of posterior activity (Mashour, 2020).
fMRI and neurofeedback experiments suggest perceptual content representations associate with posterior regions (Amano, 2017; Shibata, 2011). It remains unclear whether the representation of perceptual content is directly linked to frontal regions per se, even when reporting is necessary. Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis techniques have identified that perceptual confidence can be decoded in frontal regions (Cortese, 2017) but objective properties of stimuli seem linked to posterior regions. This research aims to determine the frontoparietal contribution to conscious visual representations and its connection with perceptual confidence.
Methods:
fMRI data from 26 participants performing the Shadlen Random Dot Motion task (Cortese, 2017) were collected. We presented a set of random dots on a screen with a percentage of dots moving in a specific direction (coherence). We asked our participants to identify movement direction (objective rating) and perceptual confidence (subjective rating). Each participant underwent at least 160 trials. Each stimulus was calibrated under coherence threshold estimated for each participant by Quest Psychtoolbox function. Trials comprised Direction, Coherence, Score, and Confidence. Two fMRI analyses were conducted: I. ROI analysis, subtracting estimated beta values for dichotomic conditions (e.g. below or above threshold). II. Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis, decoding brain activity with a Searchlight approach through The Decoding Toolbox, comparing voxel accuracy between lobes using the Desikan-Killany-Tourville atlas (Hatamimajoumerd, 2022).
Data collection and sharing for this project was provided by the DecNef Department at the Advanced Telecommunication Research Institute International, Kyoto, Japan. FONDECYT regular 1230383

Results:
ROI analysis revealed significant differences in beta ROI activity between stimuli with 25% and 75% coherence as a function of topography (p <0.05). The difference between frontal and posterior activity under low and high coherence was significantly higher. The left hemisphere also showed significant differences. Right hemisphere differences were observed between ROIs for conscious and nonconscious conditions (p=0.0257, p=0.012). The most accurate ROI for decoding movement direction below the awareness threshold included frontal regions. Significant differences in right hemisphere accuracy were observed in unconscious direction trials (p=0.023), while the left hemisphere showed differences between ROI accuracy for Score and Confidence.
Conclusions:
We expected to find high contribution when participants were aware of stimuli properties, while results suggest frontal region contribution, especially under low signal strength (low coherence). Decoding movement direction below the consciousness threshold highlighted the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) significant contribution. This suggests frontal activity in perceptual tasks relates to working memory, acting as a memory buffer under conditions requiring more time for representation. This has been observed through no-report paradigm (Hatamimajoumerd, 2022). It is also possible that conscious experience about complex stimuli may be internally driven by priors (LeDoux & Lau, 2020) or imagination when stimuli is not strong enough to surpass subjective thresholds (Dijkstra & Fleming, 2023).
Higher Cognitive Functions:
Higher Cognitive Functions Other 2
Perception, Attention and Motor Behavior:
Consciousness and Awareness 1
Keywords:
Consciousness
FUNCTIONAL MRI
Perception
1|2Indicates the priority used for review
Provide references using author date format
Amano, K., Shibata, K., Kawato, M., Sasaki, Y., & Watanabe, T. (2016). 'Learning to Associate Orientation with Color in Early Visual Areas by Associative Decoded fMRI Neurofeedback'. Curr Biol, 26(14), 1861-1866. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.014
Cortese, A., Amano, K., Koizumi, A., Lau, H., & Kawato, M. (2017). 'Decoded fMRI neurofeedback can induce bidirectional confidence changes within single participants'. Neuroimage, 149, 323-337. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.069
Del Cul, A., Baillet, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). 'Brain dynamics underlying the nonlinear threshold for access to consciousness'. PLoS biology, 5(10), e260.
Dijkstra, N., & Fleming, S. M. (2023). 'Subjective signal strength distinguishes reality from imagination'. Nature Communications, 14(1), 1627.
Hatamimajoumerd, E., Murty, N. A. R., Pitts, M., & Cohen, M. A. (2022). 'Decoding perceptual awareness across the brain with a no-report fMRI masking paradigm'. Current Biology, 32(19), 4139-4149.
LeDoux, J. E., & Lau, H. (2020). 'Seeing consciousness through the lens of memory'. Current Biology, 30(18), R1018-R1022.
Mashour, G. A., Roelfsema, P., Changeux, J. P., & Dehaene, S. (2020). 'Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis'. Neuron, 105(5), 776-798.
Searle, J. R. (2007). 'Dualism revisited'. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 101(4-6), 169-178.
Shibata, K., Watanabe, T., Sasaki, Y., & Kawato, M. (2011). 'Perceptual learning incepted by decoded fMRI neurofeedback without stimulus presentation'. Science, 334(6061), 1413-1415. doi:10.1126/science.1212003