Poster No:
1530
Submission Type:
Abstract Submission
Authors:
Stuart Oldham1, Gareth Ball1
Institutions:
1Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC
First Author:
Stuart Oldham
Murdoch Children's Research Institute
Melbourne, VIC
Co-Author:
Gareth Ball
Murdoch Children's Research Institute
Melbourne, VIC
Introduction:
Generative network models (GNMs) are used to create synthetic networks which aim to capture key properties of human brain connectomes. GNMs are parameterised using simple wiring-rules, commonly a trade-off between minimising connection cost and promoting advantageous features, such as topological value or the connection of biologically similar regions[1-3]. Differences in GNM parameters and wiring-rules may give insight into the mechanisms that shape brain connectivity. Indeed, GNM wiring constraints vary among individuals and are linked to cognition and psychopathology[4-6]. Drawing biological insight from GNMs rests on their ability to accurately model human brain networks. While GNMs capture connectome topology–including long-tailed degree distributions, clustering, and small-world properties[1-6]–they are less able to reproduce connectome topography (spatial embedding of network properties)[3]. Such embedding is critical for enabling brain dynamics, thus is a key property GNMs should capture[7,8]. Therefore, we aimed to evaluate the ability of GNMs to reproduce the topography of human brain networks.
Methods:
Diffusion tractography from 326 participants of the Human Connectome Project was used to construct a consensus 200 cortical region empirical network in the left hemisphere[9]. The GNM was formulated as a trade-off between connection cost and regional similarity of cortical metrics (Fig. 1A). Free parameters were fit using an optimisation procedure that maximised the topological similarity of the model networks to the empirical data. Networks produced using the best fit parameters were evaluated based on topological similarity, overlapping connections, and degree sequence similarity with the empirical data (Fig. 1B). Nine different GNMs, each using different cortical features[9], were evaluated (Fig. 1C).
Results:
Overall, GNMs showed high topological similarity to empirical networks (maximum dissimilarity:0.16–0.35) but reproduced only 36–44% of empirical connections and showed weak correlations with the empirical degree sequence (r=-0.48–0.18), highlighting the mismatch between synthetic and empirical topographies (Fig. 1C-1D). To isolate the underlying cause of this mismatch, we examined the overlap between connections of different lengths. GNMs were able to capture 51–84% of empirical short-range connections (<30mm), but this accuracy dropped to 2–20% for mid-range (30–90mm) and 0–10% for long-range (>90mm) connections (Fig. 2A). Empirical degree topography is mainly determined by connections >30mm (Fig. 2B). The discrepancies in spatial embedding therefore arise from GNMs' inability to accurately capture these connections. Importantly, no cortical feature was able to mitigate this error (Fig. 2C). Indeed, using a random similarity matrix achieves similar model performance to neurophysiological metrics, suggesting that the formation of long connections in GNMs is largely invariant to model input.

Conclusions:
We show that GNMs, while matching brain network topological features, fail to capture topography due to inaccurate reconstruction of mid/long-range connections. GNMs can induce topologically-similar empirical networks by mapping most short-range connections and promoting a few long-range connections, even at random. Specific long-range connections enable topographic properties that are essential to brain dynamics/function[7,8]. Drawing conclusions based on GNMs' ability to reproduce topology–but not topography–is likely to lead to an incomplete or inaccurate understanding of the mechanisms and constraints which shape brain networks. To improve GNMs, mechanisms which are preferentially biased towards long-range connectivity should be explored. Factors like connection timing may be relevant, as evidence suggests that long-range connections between hubs form earlier than other connection types[10]. Investigating these ways to improve GNMs is required so they can be used to explore the underpinning developmental mechanisms of the connectome.
Modeling and Analysis Methods:
Connectivity (eg. functional, effective, structural) 1
Neuroanatomy, Physiology, Metabolism and Neurotransmission:
Cortical Anatomy and Brain Mapping 2
Keywords:
Computational Neuroscience
Cortex
Modeling
Other - Connectome; Network; Connectivity; Generative network model; Brain maps
1|2Indicates the priority used for review
Provide references using author date format
1. Betzel, R. F., Avena-Koenigsberger, A., Goñi, J., He, Y., De Reus, M. A., Griffa, A., ... & Sporns, O. (2016). Generative models of the human connectome. Neuroimage, 124, 1054-1064.
2. Vértes, P. E., Alexander-Bloch, A. F., Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Rapoport, J. L., & Bullmore, E. T. (2012). Simple models of human brain functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(15), 5868-5873.
3. Oldham, S., Fulcher, B. D., Aquino, K., Arnatkevičiūtė, A., Paquola, C., Shishegar, R., & Fornito, A. (2022). Modeling spatial, developmental, physiological, and topological constraints on human brain connectivity. Science advances, 8(22), eabm6127.
4. Zhang, X., Braun, U., Harneit, A., Zang, Z., Geiger, L. S., Betzel, R. F., ... & Tost, H. (2021). Generative network models of altered structural brain connectivity in schizophrenia. Neuroimage, 225, 117510.
5. Akarca, D., Vértes, P. E., Bullmore, E. T., & Astle, D. E. (2021). A generative network model of neurodevelopmental diversity in structural brain organization. Nature communications, 12(1), 4216.
6. Carozza, S., Holmes, J., Vértes, P. E., Bullmore, E., Arefin, T. M., Pugliese, A., ... & Astle, D. E. (2023). Early adversity changes the economic conditions of mouse structural brain network organization. Developmental Psychobiology, 65(6), e22405. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abm6127
7. Betzel, R. F., & Bassett, D. S. (2018). Specificity and robustness of long-distance connections in weighted, interareal connectomes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(21), E4880-E4889.
8. Gollo, L. L., Roberts, J. A., Cropley, V. L., Di Biase, M. A., Pantelis, C., Zalesky, A., & Breakspear, M. (2018). Fragility and volatility of structural hubs in the human connectome. Nature Neuroscience, 21(8), 1107-1116.
9. Hansen, J. Y., Shafiei, G., Voigt, K., Liang, E. X., Cox, S. M., Leyton, M., ... & Misic, B. (2023). Integrating multimodal and multiscale connectivity blueprints of the human cerebral cortex in health and disease. Plos Biology, 21(9), e3002314.
10. Oldham, S., & Fornito, A. (2019). The development of brain network hubs. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 36, 100607.