Learning from errors: neural evidence for altered mechanisms in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Poster No:

376 

Submission Type:

Abstract Submission 

Authors:

Maria Camila Dias1,2, Teresa Sousa1,2, Susana Mouga1,3, Miguel Castelo-Branco1,2,4

Institutions:

1CIBIT - Coimbra Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Translational Research, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal, 2ICNAS - Institute for Nuclear Sciences Applied to Health, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal, 3ICNAS PHARMA Unipessoal, Lda, Ed. ICNAS, Pólo das Ciências da Saúde, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal, 4FMUC - Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal

First Author:

Maria Camila Dias  
CIBIT - Coimbra Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Translational Research, University of Coimbra|ICNAS - Institute for Nuclear Sciences Applied to Health, University of Coimbra
Coimbra, Portugal|Coimbra, Portugal

Co-Author(s):

Teresa Sousa  
CIBIT - Coimbra Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Translational Research, University of Coimbra|ICNAS - Institute for Nuclear Sciences Applied to Health, University of Coimbra
Coimbra, Portugal|Coimbra, Portugal
Susana Mouga  
CIBIT - Coimbra Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Translational Research, University of Coimbra|ICNAS PHARMA Unipessoal, Lda, Ed. ICNAS, Pólo das Ciências da Saúde, University of Coimbra
Coimbra, Portugal|Coimbra, Portugal
Miguel Castelo-Branco  
CIBIT - Coimbra Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Translational Research, University of Coimbra|ICNAS - Institute for Nuclear Sciences Applied to Health, University of Coimbra|FMUC - Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra
Coimbra, Portugal|Coimbra, Portugal|Coimbra, Portugal

Introduction:

Error monitoring involves detecting, processing, and signaling errors to prevent future mistakes. Perceiving the outcomes of our actions is vital for regulating behavior and learning (Ullsperger, Danielmeier, & Jocham, 2014). Accordingly, impaired error monitoring skills have been demonstrated in some conditions, such as Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) (Kim, Grammer, Benrey, Morrison, & Lord, 2018; Santesso et al., 2011). However, the alterations in the error monitoring neural circuitry in ASD remain to be fully understood. Here, we tested the hypothesis that the evolution of learning associated with error monitoring is altered in ASD.

Methods:

In this study, 15 non-ASD (mean age 30.7 ± 6.1 years) and 15 ASD (mean age 29.1 ± 8.7 years) male participants performed a challenging 3T functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task based on Estiveira et al. (2022). The task had multiple concurrent cues, namely emotional facial expression and gaze cues, that signaled the appropriate action and needed to be integrated to make a correct response. To understand how neural learning processes progress in error monitoring, we conducted linear mixed-effects analyses to assess the impact of group, performance, learning, and their respective interactions on key error monitoring regions, such as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and anterior insula (AI) (Dali et al., 2023; Neta et al., 2015). We also evaluated the influence of these variables on the putamen, a region linked to trial and error learning (Ashby, Turner, & Horvitz, 2010; Patterson & Knowlton, 2018). We controlled our analyses for age and Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient by including them as covariates in the linear mixed-effects models.

Results:

Behaviorally, we found a significant effect of group (F(1, 29.02) = 20.59, p = 9.10 × 10-5) and learning stage (F(6,764.19) = 34.94, p = 2.05 × 10-37) on error rate. Both groups showed clear learning curves with decreasing error rates from the beginning to the end of the session. During the entire task, the ASD group had increased error rates compared to the non-ASD group.
The fMRI results revealed that activity in the dACC and AI was modulated by the interaction between group and performance (dACC: F(2,53.23) = 10.80, p = 1.16 × 10-4; AI: F(2,54.21) = 25.48, p = 1.58 × 10-8), and learning and performance (dACC: F(12,7809.34) = 5.38, p = 3.55 × 10-9; AI: F(6,7810.24) = 7.83, p = 2.03 × 10-8). The activity in these regions was similar for correct and erroneous responses in an initial learning stage but, as learning progressed, the differences became evident. This happened due to a simultaneous decrease in activity in correct responses and an increase following errors. Although this pattern did not differ between groups, the differences between correct responses and errors were attenuated in ASD (differences being only significant in the non-ASD group). Moreover, for the ASD group, we found an inverse correlation between the difference in dACC activity between correct and erroneous responses and autistic traits (r(11) = -0.62, p = 0.023) measured by Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) total scores (Lord et al., 1989).
The putamen response was influenced by the interaction between group and learning (F(6, 7808.01) = 6.84, p = 2.99 × 10-7), and learning and performance (F(6, 7810.18) = 4.84, p = 6.10 × 10-5). In the non-ASD group, its activity increased both in correct responses and errors with learning. Nonetheless, in the ASD group, it only increased in response to errors: the activity associated with correct actions was approximately constant throughout learning.
Supporting Image: ErrorRate_run_caption2.png
Supporting Image: abstract_ohbm_fig_caption2.png
 

Conclusions:

These findings suggest that, in ASD, error monitoring mechanisms are impaired, and the learning process is altered, possibly leading to higher error rates when integrating social cues.

Emotion, Motivation and Social Neuroscience:

Emotional Perception
Emotion and Motivation Other 1

Learning and Memory:

Learning and Memory Other

Novel Imaging Acquisition Methods:

BOLD fMRI

Perception, Attention and Motor Behavior:

Perception: Visual 2

Keywords:

Addictions
Cognition
Emotions
FUNCTIONAL MRI
Learning
Limbic Systems
Perception
Other - visual perceptions

1|2Indicates the priority used for review

Provide references using author date format

Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., & Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5), 208–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.001

Dali, G., Brosnan, M., Tiego, J., Johnson, B. P., Fornito, A., Bellgrove, M. A., & Hester, R. (2023). Examining the neural correlates of error awareness in a large fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 33, 458–468. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac077

Estiveira, J., Dias, C., Costa, D., Castelhano, J., Castelo-Branco, M., & Sousa, T. (2022). An Action-Independent Role for Midfrontal Theta Activity Prior to Error Commission. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16(805080). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.805080

Kim, S. H., Grammer, J., Benrey, N., Morrison, F., & Lord, C. (2018). Stimulus processing and error monitoring in more-able kindergarteners with autism spectrum disorder: a short review and a preliminary Event-Related Potentials study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47, 556–567. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13580

Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Austism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 185–212. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02211841

Neta, M., Miezin, X. M., Nelson, S. M., Dubis, J. W., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2015). Spatial and temporal characteristics of error-related activity in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(1), 253–266. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1313-14.2015

Patterson, T. K., & Knowlton, B. J. (2018). Subregional specificity in human striatal habit learning: a meta-analytic review of the fMRI literature. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 20, 75–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.10.005

Santesso, D. L., Drmic, I. E., Jetha, M. K., Bryson, S. E., Goldberg, J. O., Hall, G. B., … Schmidt, L. A. (2011). An event-related source localization study of response monitoring and social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Psychophysiology, 48, 241–251. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01056.x

Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94, 35–79. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00041.2012